<< >> Up Title Contents Index

Brightness Sensitivity

There are several ways to describe the sensitivity of the human visual system. To begin, let us assume that a homogeneous region in an image has an intensity as a function of wavelength (color) given by I(). Further let us assume that I() = Io, a constant.

Wavelength sensitivity

The perceived intensity as a function of , the spectral sensitivity, for the "typical observer" is shown in Figure 10 .

Figure 10: Spectral Sensitivity of the "typical" human observer

Stimulus sensitivity

If the constant intensity (brightness) Io is allowed to vary then, to a good approximation, the visual response, R, is proportional to the logarithm of the intensity. This is known as the Weber-Fechner law:

The implications of this are easy to illustrate. Equal perceived steps in brightness, R = k, require that the physical brightness (the stimulus) increases exponentially. This is illustrated in Figure 11ab.

A horizontal line through the top portion of Figure 11a shows a linear increase in objective brightness (Figure 11b) but a logarithmic increase in subjective brightness. A horizontal line through the bottom portion of Figure 11a shows an exponential increase in objective brightness (Figure 11b) but a linear increase in subjective brightness.

Figure 11a Figure 11b (top) Brightness step I = k Actual brightnesses plus interpolated values (bottom) Brightness step I = k*I

The Mach band effect is visible in Figure 11a. Although the physical brightness is constant across each vertical stripe, the human observer perceives an "undershoot" and "overshoot" in brightness at what is physically a step edge. Thus, just before the step, we see a slight decrease in brightness compared to the true physical value. After the step we see a slight overshoot in brightness compared to the true physical value. The total effect is one of increased, local, perceived contrast at a step edge in brightness.

<< >> Up Title Contents Index